Treatment FAQ

why should different treatment groups be equal in number

by Joel Effertz Published 2 years ago Updated 2 years ago
image

By treating each group equally, we can conclude that any treatment effect is due to the intervention (true acupuncture) and not down to any variation in care. For example, let us instead of comparing acupuncture to sham treatment compare acupuncture to a control group of people on a back pain waiting list.

Each member of the treatment group thus has a counterpart in the control group identical in every way possible outside of the treatment. This ensures that the treatment is the only source of potential differences in outcomes between the two groups.Jul 3, 2020

Full Answer

What is the ratio of treatment to control group members?

This is because the ratio of treatment to control group members is different in different sites, 8 ranging from about 2:1 in the larger sites to 1:1 in the smaller sites.

How to estimate the treatment group mean from estimated difference?

Since the estimated difference is a weighted average of site specific treatment/ control differences, a logical choice for the estimate of the treatment group mean is to use a similar weighted average of the site treatment group means.

Why is the treatment group mean lower than the control group mean?

This would result in a treatment group mean at the model level that was lower than the control group mean, simply because the site with low ADL comprised a greater proportion of the treatment group, and in spite of the fact that the randomization process produced equivalent treatment and control groups in every site.

How is the treatment/control difference calculated?

The treatment/control difference is given by the estimate of the coefficient "a," and its standard error was used to calculate significance levels. The mean value for the treatment group was calculated as a weighted average of the individual site means for the treatment group.

image

Why is it important to have an equal number of participants in each group?

You cannot say that everyone in the population has an equal chance of being selected because you do not know the probability of selection. This is important because it means that your sample may or may not be representative of the population, and this can influence the external validity of your study.

Why is it important to keep the conditions across all groups equal?

If all groups have the same number of cases, it maximizes statistical power. The more unbalanced the groups are, the more power you lose.

Why do we compare different treatment groups?

Many studies that compare treatments will include a table comparing baseline characteristics between two groups assigned to different treatments. This allows readers to examine if there are any important baseline differences between groups.

Why should sample sizes be equal?

Unequal sample sizes can lead to: Unequal variances between samples, which affects the assumption of equal variances in tests like ANOVA. Having both unequal sample sizes and variances dramatically affects statistical power and Type I error rates (Rusticus & Lovato, 2014). A general loss of power.

Why is it important in between subjects designs to keep the different groups of participants as similar as possible?

Summary: In user research, between-groups designs reduce learning effects; repeated-measures designs require fewer participants and minimize the random noise.

What is the importance of taking repeated measurements of a single quantity?

The Benefits of Repeated Measures Designs Fewer subjects: Thanks to the greater statistical power, a repeated measures design can use fewer subjects to detect a desired effect size. Further sample size reductions are possible because each subject is involved with multiple treatments.

Why is it important to know about the difference between the means of the control group and the treatment group prior to an intervention?

Control groups help ensure the internal validity of your research. You might see a difference over time in your dependent variable in your treatment group. However, without a control group, it is difficult to know whether the change has arisen from the treatment.

What makes a good comparison group?

A good comparison group should have substantial overlap with the treatment group in terms of the characteristics likely to affect program outcomes. Informed by the logic model, the evaluator should exercise judgment regarding the key characteristics that should be used to identify similar individuals.

Why is comparing like with like important?

By ensuring our two groups have similar prognoses (comparing 'like with like'), we can increase our confidence that any difference we see is due to the treatments and not due to patient differences.

Why are unequal sample sizes bad?

The statistical results are only approximate. Unequal sample sizes result in confounding. Unequal sample sizes indicate a poor experimental design.

Do sample sizes need to be equal?

A sample size imbalance isn't a tell-tale sign of a poor study. You don't need equal-sized groups to compute accurate statistics. If the sample size imbalance is due to drop-outs rather than due to design, simple randomisation or technical glitches, this is something to take into account when interpreting the results.

Does the control group have to be the same size?

The size of the control group, or any test group for that matter, depends on the size of the total population. If the experiment is run on a population size of only 100 participants, a 5% control group would be only 5 individuals, which would certainly diminish the significance of the results.

Why is it not possible to examine all cases?

The examination of all cases is usually not done for the following reasons: it is difficult to identify all cases, it is resource intensive, and from a statistical point of view it provides little added certainty. Additionally, examination might involve destruction of the biological sample (e.g., estimating the weight of biologic organs on a scale is limited to pathologic specimens removed as warranted by surgery or at autopsy). For these reasons, the researcher is likely to use a sample in order to estimate values related to the greater population. In order to assure an accurate estimation, that sample must be representative of the population.

How does confidence interval change with sample size?

In chart 1, we can observe how the ‘confidence interval’ changes with sample size, becoming more precise with increasing sample size. When the sample size is 10,000 (row 3), the SD of the mean (SEM) is 100 times less (0.15 fL) than the SD of the sample (15 fL). By varying the sample size, the researcher can make the estimate of the mean as precise as desired.

What is inferential statistics?

When using inferential statistics, the researcher is attempting to predict an unknown value (e.g., the mean) that applies to all such patients. The true value for the entire group of all such patients is initially unknown and remains unknown. The nature of inferential statistics is the calculation of an estimate and qualification of the estimate in terms of its accuracy.

What is standard deviation?

Standard deviation is a measure of variability of the population.

Why is randomization used?

Randomization is used to diminish the likelihood of bias.

What is the true mean of a population?

As with all normal distributions, the true mean of the population will be the mean of the estimates, will be the most frequent value (mode), and will lie at the center of the distribution (median). Consequently, 50% of the estimates will lie below the true mean and 50% will lie above the true mean.

Why is it important to have a representative sample?

Because the researcher attempts to make a generalization based on the sample at hand, it is extremely important that the sample be representative of the population of interest. If the sample is not representative, then the estimated value will likely be incorrect. Errors in statistical prediction that are related to the way the study is constructed or carried out are said to be the result of ‘bias’ or ‘systematic error’. Statistical use of the word ‘bias’ does not imply any intention or prejudice. Error resulting from bias is contrasted with error resulting from mathematically predictable variability.

How are participants assigned to groups?

Most commonly, participants are assigned to groups using a computer-generated list of random numbers. Other methods include pre-defined treatment schedules and sealed envelopes with group assignments drawn at random. Again, it is very important that this random allocation occurs before the study starts (prospective allocation) to ensure parallel testing. It is also important that the allocation schedule is concealed.

What group do high risk patients end up in?

If the severe, high-risk patients are normally situated in low number rooms (closer to the nurses’ station), more severe cases will end up in group A, inflating the risk for the outcome (death) in this group.

Why is randomization important?

Randomization ensures that both groups have a similar prognosis for the outcome before the start of treatment and that any differences will be chance differences. This thus best approximates the counterfactual ideal, as described above.

What happens if a doctor is privy to this allocation scheme and believes the surgery is more effective?

If a doctor is privy to this allocation scheme and believes the surgery is more effective, he may send his sickest patients into one of the lowered numbered rooms instead of a room at random, again increasing the risk in group A.

Why is randomized controlled trial inappropriate?

Though ideal for testing treatments, for some research questions a randomized controlled trial is inappropriate due to ethical or practical concerns. For example, it would be unethical to randomize individuals to a ‘smoking’ vs. ‘no smoking’ group to test whether smoking causes cancer.

Why is it important to compare like with like?

By ensuring our two groups have similar prognoses (comparing ‘like with like’), we can increase our confidence that any difference we see is due to the treatments and not due to patient differences.

How many blogs are there on informed health choices?

This is the fourteenth blog in a series of 36 blogs based on a list of ‘Key Concepts’ developed by an Informed Health Choices project team. Each blog will explain one Key Concept that we need to understand to be able to assess treatment claims.

Why do patients switch from one treatment to another?

In many trials, some patients invariably switch from one treatment to the other owing to side effects, apparent lack of effectiveness or a simple change in preference. If researchers analyze patients based on the treatment they receive (known as per protocol or analysis by treatment administered), they risk introducing prognostic imbalances between treatment groups and lose the benefits conferred by randomization. Alternatively, the intention-to-treat approach analyzes patients in the groups to which they were randomly assigned, regardless of the treatment they actually received, and provides the least biased assessment of the efficacy of the treatment.1,20,21Intention-to-treat analysis maintains prognostic balances in study groups. In surgical trials, adherence to protocol is not usually an issue when the treatment is a one-time irreversible process, but there may be a chance of conversion from new treatment to conventional treatment for technical reasons or owing to comorbidities. The intention-to-treat analysis does not eliminate bias introduced by conversion, losses to follow-up or withdrawals, but provides the best estimate of the effect size that can be expected for patients in whom the treatment is attempted (regardless of the need for conversion).1

How long should a patient be followed?

Ideally, every patient should be followed until the completion of the study. Failure to account for all patients at the end of the study is another factor that risks introducing imbalances between treatment groups and losing the benefits conferred from randomization.1The imbalances become more prominent when there are systematic differences between comparison groups in the loss to follow-ups or drop-outs from the study. Patients who do not attend follow-up visits are usually different from the ones who do;1they may have died, experienced the outcome of interest or had a satisfactory outcome. Losses to follow-up are greater and differential when

How does randomization work?

Randomization is a process during which the patients have an equal chance of being allocated to either study treatment group. The goal is to produce comparable groups in a way that both known and unknown prognostic factors are balanced1,6and that any imbalance that might occur will be by chance rather than by choice. Randomization is the most optimal method to minimize selection bias and control for known and unknown confounding factors. A true randomization process eliminates selection bias.1The most robust and optimal method of randomization is computer-generated random numbers. Coin-tossing, dice-throwing or using random number tables (from statistical textbooks) represent reasonable approaches for the generation of simple randomization sequences, but might become nonrandom in practice. These methods do not provide concealment allocation. If, for example, using the coin-toss method, an investigator throws a series of “heads” with no “tails,” he or she might be tempted to alter the results of a toss or a series of tosses.7Some researchers allocate patients to groups in a way that is not truly random (e.g., using the day of the week or alternate medical record number) and are called “quasirandom.”1Although these methods might seem to generate comparable groups, they cannot provide concealment of allocation. This introduces a substantial risk of selection bias.1,8Using these systematic methods, the study personnel can predict to which group the next patient will be assigned and might, consciously or unconsciously, exclude that patient from the study for different reasons. To minimize bias, patients should be assigned to study groups based on a truly random process. Timing of randomization is also very important in preventing “post-randomization exclusion.” An eligible patient might become ineligible if the there is a lag time from randomization to surgical intervention. As in clinical trials, randomization should be performed very close to when the intervention is performed. If possible, patients’ informed consent should be obtained preoperatively, but randomization occurs intraoperatively once there is certainty that the patient could receive either intervention.1

Why is stratified randomization important?

Before starting the randomization sequence, the researcher should assess whether there are major prognostic factors that are strongly associated with subsequent patient response or outcome.9,11Such factors should be considered for stratified randomization. Stratified randomization prevents an imbalance between treatment groups for factors that influence treatment responsiveness. 14Stratified randomization requires the prognostic factor of interest to be measured a priori or at the time of randomization. Stratified randomization may be useful in small trials as some imbalances, for example age, might occur and complicate the interpretation of the results.7,12Within each stratum, the randomization process could be simple or restricted depending on the size of trial. In multicentre trials, centres may vary with respect to the type of patients, and the quality and type of care given to patients during follow-up. Thus, centre may be an important factor related to patient outcome, and the randomization process should be stratified accordingly.12By stratifying randomization within a centre (i.e., using separate randomization schedules at each centre), the extent to which major imbalances between treatment groups will occur across centres can be limited.11Note that the factor of blocking and/or stratifying should be taken into consideration during data analysis. The purpose of blocking and/or stratifying is to ensure balance between treatment groups and increase the power of the study; therefore, ignoring blocking and/or stratifying factors in the data analysis may result in misleading conclusions.11,15There are other randomization methods, such as the adaptive randomization process11(i.e., minimization to avoid between-group imbalances) and the maximal procedure,16details of which can be found elsewhere.

What is a high quality randomized controlled trial?

High-quality randomized controlled trials (RCTs) are the highest level of evidence in assessing the effectiveness of a treatment. It is random allocation that places RCTs in the highest level of evidence. The purpose of randomization is to create groups of patients that are comparable for known and unknown factors at the start of the trial so that any differences at the completion of the trial can be attributed to the treatment under investigation.1The purpose of this article is to discuss the processes that would help create balanced groups and maintain between-group comparability throughout the study period.

What are the criteria for surgical trials?

The inclusion criteria basically define the population of the research question. The exclusion criteria define populations of patients who will not help in answering the research question or might be harmed by research interventions. It is very important that a record is kept of all patients who were assessed for eligibility, identifying those who were excluded and stating the reason. This ensures that the risk of selection bias will be minimized (i.e., preferential exclusion of certain patients from joining the study).

Why is blinding important in surgical trials?

Therefore, researchers should make every effort to incorporate blinding into their trial designs. In trials of surgical interventions, surgeons can usually not be blinded, but patients, health care providers, data collectors, outcome assessors and data analysts can often be blinded. Blinding or masking these individuals prevents systematic imbalances in effective concomitant interventions, outcome evaluations and between-group comparability for baseline characteristics. Randomized controlled trials of surgical interventions are often more difficult to blind than drug trials, which typically achieve blinding with placebos.1It is most problematic to blind allocation from patients and research personnel when comparing a surgical intervention to nonoperative management. Group imbalances in surgical trials could occur if the outcome assessors, care providers and patients are not blinded to the treatment allocation. The outcome assessors might assess the outcome differently if they are aware of the treatment allocation. Blinding outcome assessors protects the trial against the differential assessment of the outcomes. People who set up follow-up visits may (intentionally or unintentionally) make extra efforts for complete follow-up for patients who received experimental treatment than for those who received conventional treatment if they are not blinded for treatment allocation. This may create differential follow-ups between study groups and introduce attrition bias. When patients are aware of the treatment allocation, their attendance at follow-up visits are usually different than those who are blinded to the treatment allocation. The differential loss to follow-up is greater when surgical intervention is compared with conservative management and blinding is impractical. For example, Michaels and colleagues17compared surgery to conservative management for uncomplicated varicose veins. At 1 year follow-up, there was significant attrition owing to patients failing to attend follow-up visits or withdrawing from the trial (35% conservative arm v. 17% surgery arm). By the end of the third year, 52% of patients in the conservative arm had undergone surgery. To increase the internal validity of an RCT, researchers should blind as many involved individuals as possible and clearly state which individuals are blinded and how the blinding is achieved. When blinding of patients and health care providers is not feasible, to prevent group imbalances surgical researchers should ensure that the randomization process is independently administered and that people who randomly assign patients into the trial are not involved in patient care. To maintain group comparability, the surgical researchers should ensure that the study groups are, except for the intervention, treated equally (i.e., concomitant interventions) and that every effort is made for a complete follow-up for all participating patients. Another useful tip to avoid differential assessment of outcome measures and maintain comparability between the groups when blinding is not feasible is to have 2 or more individuals independently assess outcomes and resolve the disagreements with consensus.

Why is it important to compare treatment and control groups?

The comparability of the treatment and control groups at randomization is also important because it is the first stage in our investigation of a set of methodological problems that could result in biased estimates of channeling's impact. Differences between treatment and control groups in the types of individuals who fail to respond to interviews could result in noncomparable groups in the sample being analyzed, even if the full samples were comparable. Differences in the way baseline data were collected for treatments and controls could lead to differential measurement error, which could cause regression estimates of program impacts to -be biased. In order to assess these other potential sources of bias, it is important to first determine whether the two groups were comparable before the baseline interview.

How many statistically significant differences are there between treatments and controls?

Out of over 250 comparisons at the five basic sites, we find 15 statistically significant differences between treatments and controls. (at the 90 percent or greater confidence level). This is substantially less than the 25 that might be expected to occur simply by chance. As shown in Table 4, the significant differences were more prevalent in Kentucky than in other sites, but tended to be scattered rather than concentrated in specific variables. Thus, there is no indication of systematic tampering with the random assignment process.

What are the factors that lead to differences in the mean values of the pre-application characteristics of the treatment and control groups?

Only two factors can lead to differences in the true mean values of the pre-application characteristics of the treatment and control groups: deviation from the randomization procedures and normal sampling variability. Deviations from the carefully developed randomization procedures could be either deliberate (e.g., site staff purposely misrecording as treatments some applicants who are randomly assigned to the control group, but who have especially pressing needs for assistance) or accidental (e.g., misrecording of a sample member's status). The dedication and professionalism of this site staff and the safeguards built into the assignment procedure make either occurrence very unlikely. Site staff were extremely cooperative in faithfully executing the procedures. Sampling variability, on the other hand, is the difference between the two groups that occurs simply by chance. For the sample sizes available at the model level, such differences between the two groups should be very small, and statistically insignificant.

Why was the screening instrument used in the Channeling project?

The screening instrument was designed for a short telephone interview, to be administered in a uniform manner by each of the 10 demonstration projects. The telephone screening process was intended to reduce the cost of determining appropriateness for channeling compared to using a comprehensive in-person assessment for that purpose. Channeling project staff who conducted the screening interviews were in a separate administrative unit from assessment and case management staff. This was required chiefly to preserve the integrity of the experimental design--the potential for influencing the behavior of persons assigned to the control groups through contact with channeling staff was minimized by this administrative separation.

Why is treatment/control difference statistically tested?

However, because of the relatively small number of observations at each site, most of the analysis of channeling will be based on treatment/control differences at the model level, to ensure a high level of precision (i.e., the ability to distinguish between fairly small impacts of channeling and differences between treatment and control groups arising simply by chance).

How are treatments different from controls?

Demographics and living arrangements show no significant differences between treatments and controls for the financial control model. Slightly more treatments than controls are male; slightly more controls than treatments are black. The proportion of treatments with income in excess of 1,000 dollars per month was significantly lower for treatments than controls (5.7 versus 7.3 percent, respectively); however, the difference is not large in absolute terms and the average incomes of the two groups do not differ significantly. Just over 2 percent of both treatments and controls lived in long term care institutions at the time the screen.

Is there a difference between treatment and control?

There is very little difference between treatments and controls in the basic case management model. Of the 53 variables examined in Table 3, the only statistically significant difference between treatments and controls was in the proportion of referrals from case management agencies. Treatment/control differences tended to be small in relation to the mean for the treatment group, with very low test statistics. Furthermore, a joint test that the multiple correlation' between treatment/control status and all of the variables (controlling for site) is zero could not be rejected. 11

How many Q&A communities are there on Stack Exchange?

Stack Exchange network consists of 178 Q&A communities including Stack Overflow, the largest, most trusted online community for developers to learn, share their knowledge, and build their careers.

What is cross validated?

Cross Validated is a question and answer site for people interested in statistics, machine learning, data analysis, data mining, and data visualization. It only takes a minute to sign up.

What is the benefit of the former model?

Note, the latter DiD model only considers the subset of control/low intensity observations. In sum, you can do it both ways. The benefit of the former model is it allows you to get the job done in one shot.

Is TREAT1 a Cgroup?

To code 3 exposure levels, you need two contrasts. TREAT1 is a comparisonal group, but TREAT2 is also listed as a cgroup. Thus assuming TREAT1 and TREAT2 is for low and high exposure, the control is the reference.

Can you fit homoscedasticity in one model?

You can fit it in one model if the homoscedasticity assumption holds or other adjustments are made

What is resampling in statistics?

A resampling method such as bootstrapping might be used. Bootstrapping is any test or metric that uses "random sampling with replacement" (e.g., see https://www.statisticshowto.com/resampling-techniques/ ). A bootstrapping procedure could be used to draw a random sub-sample of 70 participants from the 140 participants in the control group, perform statistical analysis based on this subsample, put the 70 participants back into the control group (i.e., replacement), draw a different random sub-sample of 70 participants from the control group, perform statistical analysis again, put the 70 participants back into the control group, and perform this process repeatedly. The mean from the distribution of these estimated statistics across all of these repeated samples provides the most valid representation of the effect of the control group across the 140 participants because it does not privilege the control group in any of these re-estimations with excessive statistical power from having twice the number of participants as the experimental group, while at the same time, the information from the 140 participants in the control group still contribute to the analyses.

Can you have unequal sample sizes?

Unequal sample sizes: It is feasible to have unequal sample sizes, i.e. up to two times in the control group, in an intervention/experimental study like in observational studies. However, for your study in older persons you need to find evidence to support that the effect of your new intervention (i.e.

What are the causes of unequal sample sizes?

There are three main causes of unequal sample sizes: simple random assignment of participants to conditions; planned imbalances; and drop-outs and missing data. I will discuss these in order.

What is usually (though not invariably) of interest in a factorial design?

What’s usually (though not invariably) of interest in a factorial design is the interaction between the predictors rather than their main effects.

How to do random assignment?

Random assignment can be accomplished in essentially two different ways. The first technique is complete randomisation : first, sixty participants are recruited; then, half of them are randomly assigned to the control and half to the experimental condition. This technique guarantees that an equal number of participants is assigned to both conditions. The second technique is simple randomisation: for each participant that volunteers for the experiment, there’s a 50/50 chance that she ends up in the control or in the experimental condition – regardless of how large either sample already is. Simple randomisation causes unequal sample sizes: you’re not guaranteed to get exactly 30 heads in 60 coin flips, and similarly you’re not guaranteed to get exactly 30 participants in either condition.

How much power does randomization have?

On average, an experiment in which 60 participants are assigned to the conditions according to a simple randomisation procedure has 75% power to detect a difference of 0.7 standard deviations. The difference in power between complete randomisation (guaranteeing equal sample sizes) and simple randomisation, then, is minimal. Table 1 compares a couple of additional set-ups, and all point to the same conclusion: the loss of power associated with simple vs. complete randomisation is negligible.

Is simple randomisation better than complete randomisation?

30/30, complete randomisation) than if they’re distributed unevenly (e.g. 20/40). (This is assuming that the variability in both conditions is comparable.) For this reason, it’s usually much better to have 50 participants in both conditions rather than 20 in one condition and 200 in the other – even though the total number of participants is much greater in the second set-up. Simple randomisation, however, can cause such imbalances. In fact, it’s possible to end up with no participants in one of the groups.

Can randomisation cause imbalances?

Simple randomisation, however, can cause such imbalances. In fact, it’s possible to end up with no participants in one of the groups. But. While stark imbalances are possible when using simple randomisation, they’re also pretty improbable.

Is sample size imbalance a sign of poor study?

A sample size imbalance isn’t a tell-tale sign of a poor study.

image
A B C D E F G H I J K L M N O P Q R S T U V W X Y Z 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9